Eastern Chimpanzee, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii
EASTERN CHIMPANZEE
Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii

Geographic Distribution and Habitat
As their name suggests, Eastern chimpanzees are found in the eastern part of equatorial Africa. From north to south, their range extends from the southeastern CAR (Central African Republic) through northern DRC (Democratic Republic of Congo). The Congo River, a significant geographical feature, serves as their southern limit. In the East, their range includes southeastern Sudan, Uganda, Rwanda, Burundi, and Tanzania. The best-studied populations of Eastern chimpanzees are from Tanzania, where Dr. Jane Goodall conducted her research for over 30 years.
Eastern chimpanzees live in different habitats, from evergreen rainforest swamps to savanna grassland and dry woodlands.
They are sometimes known as the long-haired chimpanzee.
Eastern chimpanzees are a subspecies of the common chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). In 2005, there was a proposal to divide the existing taxonomy of Eastern chimpanzees into two different subspecies. Some researchers have noted that in northern DRC, populations of Eastern chimpanzees have significantly larger skulls and limbs than populations in the southeastern part of their distribution. However, the landscape lacks a physical barrier (such as a mountain range or river) to separate these two populations and prevent hybridization or mating. Without further genetic studies, confirming whether the anatomical variations are consistent enough to classify the two populations as separate species is difficult. Currently, there is no taxonomic subdivision of Eastern chimpanzees.

Size, Weight, and Lifespan
The Eastern chimpanzee is the smallest of the common chimpanzees. Males weigh about 88 pounds (40 kg), and females weigh 66 pounds (30kg). Both sexes are about the same standing height of 3.3 to 5.7 feet (1.1 to 1.7 m). Sexual dimorphism (visual differences between males and females) is mainly through size because male chimpanzees are significantly heavier and more muscular than females. Females can have visibly conspicuous swelling of their reproductive organs, but this is only when they are hormonally ready to mate.
Chimpanzees can live for 40 to 45 years in the wild. However, infant mortality is high in chimpanzees, and a newborn’s life expectancy is only about 15 years. If young chimpanzees survive past puberty, they usually live to be about 40 to 45 years old. In captivity, they can live to be 60 years old.
Appearance
Chimpanzees are covered in dark hair that is usually black but can range in shades of brown. Their face, hands, feet, and patches near the reproductive organs remain hairless. Eastern chimpanzees are smaller and have rounded heads with a narrower brow ridge than other chimp subspecies. They have prominent, rounded ears that stick out the sides of their head. Their arms are much longer than their bodies, leading to their typical knuckle-walking on all four limbs. They have opposable thumbs and big toes for gripping branches and food items.
As they age, chimpanzees lose the pigment in their hair and can have patches of white hair or even lose their hair and develop bald spots. Infants are born with dark fur and a pale tail-tuft that becomes dark as they age. The tail tuft is an easy way to estimate the age of young chimps.

Diet
Chimpanzees are omnivorous and have a diet that varies between leaves, fruits, tree sap, bark, insects, eggs, birds, and mammals such as squirrels and other primates. They prefer ripe fruit and travel far to eat from a favorite tree. Eastern chimpanzees live in varied habitats, so their diet reflects the food available in these areas. For example, chimps in savannahs have a less varied diet and less fruit than chimps living in rainforests. An average male eats about 1900 calories in Tanzania’s Gombe National Park. Some chimps have been observed “self-medicating,” eating leaves of certain plants to fight gut parasites such as tapeworms. This plasticity in their diet allows them to adapt and survive fluctuations in food availability.
Chimpanzees are famous for actively hunting and preying on red colobus monkeys (Piliocolobus badius). Chimpanzee hunting parties usually consist of multiple males who chase and flank their prey through the canopy. The spoils of the hunt are shared between members of the hunting party. Chimps hunt throughout the seasons, even when fruits are abundant. So, it is still unclear why chimpanzees choose to hunt, which is a potentially dangerous and energetically costly behavior. Some suggest that meat provides chimpanzees with essential minerals and salts that plant matter does not. For chimpanzees, the protein-rich rewards of hunting may be worth the risk of injury and sharing a meal with others.
Behavior and Lifestyle
Chimpanzees travel by walking on all fours (quadrupedal), using their knuckles to support their body weight. Sometimes, they walk bipedally, upright on two legs. Bipedal walking is less stable because of the angle of the thigh bones in their hip sockets, and it cannot be done for a long time. They spend about the same time traveling through trees (arboreal) and on the ground (terrestrial). They are active only during the day (diurnal) and sleep in groups in nests for the night.
Chimpanzees are considered one of our closest relatives. For years, behavioral studies on chimpanzees have been conducted to better understand human evolution, especially regarding sociality, intelligence, and culture. Chimpanzee behavior has been a subject of many publications because they have been kept in captivity at zoos or research facilities for decades. It is essential to note that many behaviors (e.g., how chimpanzees eat, share meals, and socially interact) depend on the chimpanzee’s environment, so captive behaviors differ significantly from wild chimpanzees. Wild and captive chimpanzees are aggressive and prone to fights, especially between unfamiliar or non-family troops. However, after a fight, wild chimpanzees are unlikely to reconcile or “make peace” with those they fought with. However, in captivity, reconciliatory behavior has been observed, most likely because different groups are forced to live close to each other, and one group cannot move away. Below, we will focus on behaviors commonly seen in wild chimpanzees.
Chimpanzees are one of the great apes that make nests. They mostly build their nests in trees at 36 to 52 feet (11 to 16 m). Nests act as mattresses, usually made from a bed of twigs and branches and softer leaves on top. Once they are weaned from their mother, all chimpanzees gather leaves and materials to build their nests to sleep in at night. New nests are often built every night, and if a nest is re-used, chimpanzees will still add more material to rebuild and modify the old nest. Two or more chimpanzees can join in building individual nests or a shared nest in a “nest party.” Nests can be built at any time of day. Daytime nests are roughly built and used only as a temporary resting site between feeding bouts. Nighttime nests are sturdier and are used to sleep in throughout the night. Nests constructed during the daytime tend to be higher in the trees than those built at night. Adult female chimpanzees tend to build their nests higher than males. Though scientists have not confirmed the reasons for the difference in nest heights between males and females, they have suggested some hypotheses. It may be that males are too heavy to successfully and safely build nests in the higher branches of trees. Males are also the primary protectors of the troop against predators, and building their nests close to the ground where predators like lions may attack is the best strategy for protecting the family.
There is considerable variation in hunting behavior between chimpanzee populations across their range. One hypothesis suggested that chimpanzees hunted and offered meat to females to gain favor during their mating period. However, in some parties, the presence of females in estrus did not increase the number of hunts males participated in, which indicates that males do not exchange hunting favors for mating access. Some studies noted that not every hunting party member received a fair share of the hunt, which suggests that social bonds between males do not necessarily benefit from hunting together. Across populations, chimpanzees are opportunistic hunters even when fruits are abundant. They hunt more in open habitats such as semideciduous forests, where prey are more visible than denser evergreen forests. Ecological factors rather than social ones are better drivers for predicting when chimpanzees hunt.
Tool use has been famously documented in chimpanzee communities, especially ant-dipping, where chimpanzees use a stick to dip into a termite or ant nest to collect the insects to eat. Some field studies have reported advanced tool-set use, where chimpanzees use a branch or rock to break the nest and a stick or stem to scoop the insects out. These multi-step problem-solving techniques are rare abilities in the animal world.
Researchers have also noted that the tools used by chimpanzees differ by region. In some areas, chimpanzees prefer to use different stick lengths to “dip” for different ant species. While in other places, they do not use tools to break the termite or ant nest and only use a stem to dip and pick up ants. Tool use is learned through trial and error and taught from one individual to another. So, a particular technique is learned and passed on to the younger generation of a troop in a specific region. Because female chimpanzees disperse from their natal troop, females likely teach these techniques to troops in other areas. In this manner, tool-use techniques are spread across space. The longer a method is used and the more it is tried in new environments, the higher the probability that the chimpanzee tries and succeeds in improving the tool use or method for their needs.
Learning through watching other members of your community and teaching behaviors to the next generation is the primary step in developing a culture in a community. Culture was once considered a uniquely human characteristic because of our ability to learn and teach each other through social interactions. Chimpanzees also learn socially and can transfer learned behaviors to the next generation. Many researchers accept that these learned behaviors show the development of culture in chimpanzee communities. Chimpanzee behaviors taught across generations include tool use (e.g., what stick lengths are best to get different ant species), gestures, foraging behaviors (what food to avoid or tools to use), and even digging wells for water. These chimpanzee behaviors differ in different communities and regions and point towards cultural diversity in the species. From archeological and historical texts that chronicle observations of chimpanzees, we know that some of these behaviors have persisted for centuries, indicating the evolutionary advantage of maintaining these behaviors.
Grooming is an important part of chimpanzee behavior and life in general. As social animals, transferring pests such as fleas between individuals is common. Grooming is an essential part of maintaining hygiene in the group. The act of grooming is advantageous for both the groomed and the groomer. The groomed gets cleaned, and the groomer gets to snack on any pests they find. There is a social aspect of grooming where kinship between groomer and groomed increases during the behavior. Much like how a mother combing her child’s hair is a sign of love and trust between the two, grooming in primates is also an act of affinity or trust that strengthens the bond between the two individuals. In Mahale, Tanzania, grooming behaviors have developed into cultural behavior where a pair of chimpanzees clasp each other’s hands over their heads and groom each other with their free hand. This behavior is only seen in a few communities and is uncommon for all chimpanzees.
An interesting outcome of social and cultural learning seen in chimpanzees is the unique events of Tanzania’s Gombe National Park. Two chimpanzee communities engaged in a multi-year conflict that many researchers (including the eminent chimpanzee expert Dr. Jane Goodall) compare to the equivalent of war in human culture. This phenomenon is called the “Gombe chimpanzee war” or the “four-year war” that occurred between 1974 and 1978.
The Gombe war was not just the aggressive fights commonly seen among male chimpanzees. Long-term observations of the conflict showed a clear battle for power and resources, similar to human politics. The level of violence sets this conflict apart from the other aggressive fights or hunts seen in the species. Members were ruthless, twisting limbs and bodily harming their victims even after their deaths.
The Gombe War started with the relatively peaceful Kasakela community and the death of a senior male, which led to the community fractioning into the Kasakela and southern Kahama groups. A chimpanzee named “Humphrey” took over the “alpha” role in the Kasakela community, which was highly contested by a pair of brothers named “Hugh” and “Charlie.” The Kasakela community was divided into two groups depending on their allegiance, Humphrey leading the Kasakela group and the brothers leading the southern Kahama group. Neither group socialized with the other and split their space into two territories. On January 7, 1974, six Kasakela males killed a lone southern Kahama male without any provocation. This event triggered a series of vicious killings of males and females in the group of south Kahama by Kasakela members. Over the four years, there was a systematic eradication of all the males of the southern Kahama group and a progressive territorial takeover by the Kasakela group. Surviving southern Kahama females, juveniles, and less dominant males were absorbed into the Kasakela group until only one group emerged victoriously and controlled the entire territory. Researchers were shocked at the violence and domination by members who had essentially played and grown up with their victims. Since the documentation of the Gombe chimpanzee war, researchers have noted similar conflicts in other chimpanzee communities. This phenomenon raises interesting questions about the evolution of warfare in humans and the deep-rooted triggers for violence.
Eastern chimpanzees are great apes that live in troops with as many as 150 individuals.
Male chimps hunt together to catch prey like the red colobus monkey.
They can learn by observing others, and behaviors such as tool use can be passed on from generation to generation.
Their social learning develops cultures that have persisted in populations for hundreds of years.
The Gombe chimpanzee war is the first documented violent conflict between two chimpanzee groups that is thought to be similar to warfare seen in human society.
Chimpanzees are highly social animals, forming large troops with up to 150 individuals. They have a fission-fusion grouping system with smaller, temporary groups called “parties” within the larger troop. The size of the party may depend on the amount of food available, with larger parties forming during the fruiting season when food is abundant.
Male chimpanzees are gregarious and form large cooperative groups that feed and protect the females and young together. Males stay in the troops they were born in, so male groups are usually related. They have linear hierarchies where one male dominates the others, primarily competing over female access for mating. Even among the subordinate males, some have higher social importance than other males. The general terms “alpha,” “beta,” and “gamma” have been applied to this hierarchy, with alpha signifying the most dominant and gamma the least dominant of the dominant males. However, these roles are not fixed, and individuals can lose their alpha designation through fights with other males. The second dominant or beta male often attacks the alpha to usurp his position. Sometimes, a gamma male who supports the alpha in competitions will switch his support or allegiance to the beta male. This strategy, termed “allegiance fickleness,” gives gamma males the best chance of climbing up the hierarchy, especially if they think the beta male has the upper hand during a fight.
Females are less gregarious than males and form groups of unrelated females and their young. Females also exhibit hierarchies among other females, though their groups are smaller and more peaceful than males. The rarity of disputes in female groups can make it challenging to determine which females are more or less dominant. The primary source of competition for females is food, especially for high-value foods like ripe fruits. Females usually form small cliques with a high tolerance for each other and feed next to each other. Some aggressive behaviors, such as pant-grunts, have been observed if multiple cliques congregate over a limited food source. But generally, cliques avoid confrontation. Most aggressive behaviors are observed when unknown parties or female groups enter essential feeding areas.
Chimpanzees are diurnal (active during the day) and spend most of their time foraging for food. They can spend 8–20% of their day traveling. Chimpanzees usually forage in their smaller parties. On average, they move about 0.6 miles (1 km per day) and tend to travel more during drier months. Their movement is determined mainly by seasonal food availability and temperature. Trees bear fewer fruits during drier seasons, so chimps must travel further to forage. During the hotter dry season, they travel more by ground because it is easier and cooler. Tree leaves and branches trap a lot of heat, and the temperature increases higher up in the canopy. So, in the rainy season, chimpanzees spend more time in the tree canopy, where they can stay warmer and drier.
Between feeding bouts, they rest during the day, when young chimps play with each other. Sometimes, they make rough nests to lie down in for the afternoon. After their rest, chimps move along to the next forage spots. Males travel more than females and usually continue ahead and patrol territorial boundaries. In contrast, females may return to a fruiting tree to forage more. Before sunset, the party gets together again and builds their nests where they will sleep for the night.
Chimpanzees have a vast repertoire of communication methods, including facial and hand gestures, vocal sounds, and olfactory (scent) signals.
Vocalization is essential to communicate in social animals that disperse and come together across distances like chimpanzees do in their fission-fusion social system. Chimps use their voices to communicate over long and short distances, and they learn from each other and develop vocal dialects as part of their local culture. These vocal dialects can be considered “slang” that develops through talking (vocalizing) with each other over a number of years.
Thirteen broad call types have been studied in chimpanzees, such as grunts, hoos, panted hoos, barks, screams, whimpers, etc. These calls are used in combinations and social contexts to elicit specific behavior, such as a scream alarm call that makes other chimpanzees stop and become more alert. Loud calls (pant hoots, pant barks, and screams) are usually made when party members are dispersed, and they need to communicate over long distances. Rough grunt calls are typically associated with communication if a food source is nearby. Screams are associated with aggression from other chimpanzees. These screams are sometimes used to intimidate or respond to being injured. Even though these screams may sound similar to our ears, studies using playback recordings have shown that chimpanzees can tell whether a scream is caused by pain even if they cannot see the individual who made it.
Gestures make up only 4% of how chimps communicate with each other but have many commonalities with how humans gesture. Chimpanzees often face each other and take turns gesturing to each other, similar to how humans converse. These gestures can be mixed with actual contact, like tapping the other’s hand. They also use gestures during grooming sessions to indicate where they may have an itch or need special attention.
Facial expressions are used for close-contact communications. A “full closed grin” showing their teeth signals fear. It can trigger fear in other chimpanzees, especially if they are less dominant. Playfulness is associated with open-mouthed grins. They also pout, sneer, and bob their heads to show when they are upset, threatened, and submissive, respectively.
Chimpanzees have relatively long lives (about 40 years or so), and their development from infant to adult is slow (about 15 years). The gestation period of a pregnant chimpanzee (the time she is pregnant) is about 7.5 months. When infants are born, they are carried across their mother’s chest constantly for at least the first 30 days. Newborns are entirely dependent on their mother for survival and have only the essential muscle development to cling on to their mother. When infants are about 5 months old, they move to ride on their mother’s back. When infants reach around two years, they show small degrees of independence by clinging to their mothers less and sitting a few feet away from them. Infants depend on their mother’s milk until they are about 4 or 6 years old, though some solid food may be introduced before then.
Males reach puberty at 9 to 15 years old, and females at about 10. Males stay in their natal troop (the community they were born in). Females usually disperse from their natal troop before they become adults to prevent breeding with close relatives. When females go through their first menarche (first menstrual period), they also have the characteristic swelling of the anogenital region (where reproductive organs are located) seen in adult females when they are ready to mate. These adolescent females are usually infertile to prevent accidental inbreeding pregnancies with males in the troop. During this phase, she will emigrate or leave her natal troop in search of a new troop where she can mate and start a family with unrelated males.
Young adult females in the new group take some time to establish themselves. They usually have their first child when they are about 13 years old. She usually gives birth to single offspring and raises them for 3 to 5 years before becoming pregnant again.
Mating in chimpanzees can occur at any time of the year and generally occurs when the female is hormonally ready, a condition called estrus. When she is in estrus, her external reproductive organs (genitalia or anogenital region) swell, which is a visual signal to males that she is ready to mate, and mating will usually lead to a pregnancy. During estrus, females mate with multiple males, so they are promiscuous when in estrus.
Years of observations have revealed three patterns in their mating system– opportunistic, possessive, and consortship. In the opportunistic system, there is no competition among males; they mate with a ready female without any antagonistic event. In contrast, in the possessive system, a male bonds with a female in estrus and actively defends her from lower-ranking chimpanzees trying to mate her. This system is usually associated with more fights between males, and only one male will mate with a female. In consortships, a male and female will temporarily leave the troop and mate during this time while avoiding other troop members. Interestingly, male rank or dominance over females in Eastern chimpanzees is not the best predictor of whether a male enters a possessive or consortship mating system. More dominant males can fight off other males, but when two males are similarly matched, females use their social behaviors as favorable measures to develop a mating bond. The level of relationship he develops with a female determines his chances of getting exclusive access to her when she is in estrus. So, the time he spends around females in estrus, the time he spends grooming these females, and how often he shares his food with them gives him a better chance of mating with her in a possessive or consortship system. One 22-year study found that younger and low-ranking males fathered more offspring than expected, though dominant males sired the most offspring.

Chimpanzees are charismatic primates of their habitat, providing valuable insight into the evolution of human behaviors and traits. The chimpanzee’s ability to adapt to changing food availability, solve problems of getting nutrient-rich foods (such as ants and honey or digging water wells), and socially transfer their behaviors across generations are aspects of their natural history that can help us understand complex subjects such as cognition and culture.
Beyond the selfish human-centric values that chimpanzees have that help us understand our world, chimpanzees have an intrinsic value to their habitat, where the survival of the environment and chimpanzees are linked. Chimpanzees travel seasonally, feeding on fruits in large quantities. They discard seeds far away from the parent tree, contributing to new tree growth and regeneration of the forest. They travel in large groups and can cause breakage of branches and clearing of forest paths. At first glance, this may seem destructive, but this behavior promotes new growth of the smaller bushes and creates paths for other wildlife to travel through. Breaking branches create light gaps between canopies and provide valuable sunlight to smaller plants that would get shaded out by taller trees.
Chimpanzees are also part of the complex food network of their habitat. Lions and leopards prey on chimpanzees, especially young ones, and help control the populations of these primates that can live in large groups and consume a lot of fruit. Similarly, chimpanzees help control the population of beetles and ants that overrun an ecosystem if it were not for predators like chimps.


The International Union for Conservation of Nature lists the Eastern chimpanzee as Endangered (IUCN, 2016), appearing on the IUCN Red List of Threatened Species.
The eastern chimpanzee population is decreasing across their range. As a country’s economic development progresses, roads and human settlements (cities, towns, shops, agricultural farms, etc.) inevitably increase. African countries have seen an increase in their industrialization and infrastructure. As a result, forests are cut down and fragmented, which hurts wildlife. Human development also brings pollution, particularly of rivers, which has a cascading impact on the quantity and quality of water resources for wild plants and animals. In areas where chimpanzees live close to human settlements, researchers have found human food and waste sources in the guts of chimpanzees. The chances of diseases transmitted from or to humans are higher when humans encroach into forest habitats.
Logging forests and developing roads also make it easier for poachers to access chimpanzees, which are relatively large and prized in the bushmeat trade. Some local cultures are averse to eating chimpanzee meat, but researchers have found an increase in the number of chimp carcasses and orphans that are found in illegal bushmeat markets. Chimpanzee meat is a luxury in some international markets, and a hunter can get much money for a single chimp.
Recently, chimpanzees have become a tourist attraction, and people are guided into forests to view chimpanzees in their habitat. These “eco-tourism” excursions can help generate monetary value for wildlife and habitats. Still, they also increase contamination of these natural areas and the potential for transmitting a novel (new) disease to the animals. Some examples of human-origin pathogens (disease-causing microorganisms) that have been found in chimpanzees are the common cold virus (rhinovirus C), human metapneumovirus (HMPV), and intestinal parasites such as Cryptosporidium parasite.
The chimpanzee’s intelligent and human-like mannerisms make them coveted objects of the pet trade, particularly infant chimpanzees that are easier to control. Unfortunately, none of these processes have had any positive outcomes for the chimpanzee. Infant chimpanzees are usually orphans from hunted mothers who are killed for bushmeat or to capture the babies. Captured chimpanzees that are social and need a family to develop often die in transport. If these infants survive adolescence, these chimpanzees are much stronger than adult humans, which leads to human injuries and the eventual killing of the captured, “unmanageable” wild animal. The devastating outcomes of the chimpanzee pet trade are the most provocative argument against the industry because of the chimpanzees’ human-like nature.
Eastern chimpanzee is listed in Appendix I of the Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species (CITES), an international agreement between governments whose goal is to ensure that international trade in specimens of wild animals and plants does not threaten their survival.
As charismatic primates with human-like qualities, chimpanzees have been the subject of many conservation movements and documentaries, gaining public interest in their conservation. However, their survival depends on complex political situations (including war), economic insecurity, and environmental issues such as deforestation and climate change.
Throughout their range, chimpanzee habitat is protected in patches through national parks in their respective countries. However, preventing hunting and preserving habitat outside of these parks is impractical. For example, in Tanzania, where most of the Eastern chimpanzee research has been conducted, almost 75% of chimpanzees live outside of national parks, where they are exposed to hunting and degraded food quality. Still, parks like the Mahale Mountain National Park in Tanzania and Nyungwe National Park in Rwanda are a critical haven for chimpanzees, protected from human encroachment. Tracts of protected land must be connected to allow chimpanzees the space and food resources to survive human development. More collaborative work between locals, conservationists, and the government needs to occur to maintain habitat corridors that allow wildlife to move through existing habitats without the threat of hunting or accidental killing (e.g., road accidents).
The IUCN Action Plan was created to reduce the decline of eastern chimpanzee populations. The plan includes identifying habitats that can be protected and have the most significant impact on chimpanzee survival.
The IUCN A.P.E.S (Ape, Populations, Environments, and Surveys) database supports research and storage of data on the great apes. They are essential for disseminating information about the great apes, such as chimpanzees, to the public and conservationists.
- Arnold, K. and Whiten, A., 2001. Post-conflict behaviour of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Behaviour, 138(5), pp.649-690.
- Badihi, G., Graham, K.E., Grund, C., Safryghin, A., Soldati, A., Donnellan, E., Hashimoto, C., Mine, J.G., Piel, A.K., Stewart, F. and Slocombe, K.E., 2024. Chimpanzee gestural exchanges share temporal structure with human language. Current Biology, 34(14), pp.R673-R674.
- Brownlow, A.R., Plumptre, A.J., Reynolds, V. and Ward, R., 2001. Sources of variation in the nesting behavior of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. American Journal of Primatology: Official Journal of the American Society of Primatologists, 55(1), pp.49-55.
- Chitayat, A. B., Wich, S. A., Lewis, M., Stewart, F. A., & Piel, A. K. (2021). Ecological correlates of chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) density in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. PLoS ONE, 16(2), e0246628. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0246628
- Estes, R.D., 1999. The safari companion: a guide to watching African mammals including hoofed mammals, carnivores, and primates. Chelsea Green Publishing.
- Fraser, B. (2018, September 15). Taste for gorilla and Chimp Meat Fuels Illicit Trade – CIFOR-ICRAF Forests News. CIFOR. https://forestsnews.cifor.org/57897/taste-for-gorilla-and-chimp-meat-fuels-illicit-trade?fnl=en#:~:text=Many%20people%20living%20in%20rural,sometimes%20used%20in%20traditional%20medicine.
- Gerstner, K. F., & Pruetz, J. D. (2022). Wild Chimpanzee Welfare: A Focus on Nutrition, Foraging and Health to Inform Great Ape Welfare in the Wild and in Captivity. Animals : An Open Access Journal From MDPI, 12(23), 3370. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12233370
- Gilby, I.C., Eberly, L.E., Pintea, L. and Pusey, A.E., 2006. Ecological and social influences on the hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Animal Behaviour, 72(1), pp.169-180.
- Giuliano, C., Stewart, F. A., & Piel, A. K. (2022). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) grouping patterns in an open and dry savanna landscape, Issa Valley, western Tanzania. Journal of Human Evolution, 163, 103137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2021.103137
- Guardian News and Media. (2010, September 7). Congolese chimpanzees face new “wave of killing” for bushmeat. The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/environment/2010/sep/07/congo-chimpanzees-bushmeat
- Hale, T. (2024, November 19). Gombe Chimpanzee war: A 4-year power struggle in the forests of Tanzania. IFLScience. https://www.iflscience.com/gombe-chimpanzee-war-a-4-year-power-struggle-in-the-forests-of-tanzania-76840
Hashimoto C, Isaji M, Koops K, Furuichi T. First records of tool-set use for ant-dipping by Eastern chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Kalinzu Forest Reserve, Uganda. Primates. 2015 Oct;56(4):301-5. doi: 10.1007/s10329-015-0478-y. Epub 2015 Aug 5. PMID: 26243503. - Hicks, T. C., Kühl, H. S., Boesch, C., Dieguez, P., Ayimisin, A. E., Fernandez, R. M., … & Hart, J. (2019). Bili-Uéré: A Chimpanzee Behavioural Realm in Northern Democratic Republic of Congo. Folia Primatologica, 90(1), 3-64. doi:10.1159/000492998
- Hill, K., Boesch, C., Goodall, J., Pusey, A., Williams, J. and Wrangham, R., 2001. Mortality rates among wild chimpanzees. Journal of human evolution, 40(5), pp.437-450.
- IUCN PSG SGA. (2021). Chimpanzee cultures. Working Group on Chimpanzee Cultures. https://www.iucngreatapes.org/chimpanzee-cultures
- Kahlenberg, S.M., Emery Thompson, M. and Wrangham, R.W., 2008. Female competition over core areas in Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 29, pp.931-947.
- Mitani, J.C. and Watts, D.P., 2001. Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat?. Animal Behaviour, 61(5), pp.915-924.
- Moore, J.F., Mulindahabi, F., Gatorano, G., Niyigaba, P., Ndikubwimana, I., Cipolletta, C. and Masozera, M.K., 2018. Shifting through the forest: home range, movement patterns, and diet of the eastern chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in Nyungwe National Park, Rwanda. American journal of primatology, 80(8), p.e22897.
- Nishida, T. and Uehara, S., 1983. Natural diet of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): long-term record from the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. African Study Monographs, 3, pp.109-130.
- Nishida, T., 1983. Alpha status and agonistic alliance in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Primates, 24, pp.318-336.
- Pascual-Garrido, A. (2019). Cultural variation between neighbouring communities of chimpanzees at Gombe, Tanzania. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44703-4
- Péter, H., Zuberbühler, K. & Hobaiter, C. Well-digging in a community of forest-living wild East African chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Primates 63, 355–364 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-022-00992-4
- Plumptre, A.J., 2010. Eastern chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): status survey and conservation action plan, 2010-2020. IUCN.
- Plumptre, A., Hart, J.A., Hicks, T.C., Nixon, S., Piel, A.K. & Pintea, L. 2016. Pan troglodytes ssp. schweinfurthii (errata version published in 2016). The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2016: e.T15937A102329417.
- Rozenbaum, Mia., 2019. Human diseases are threatening chimpanzees. (n.d.). Understanding Animal Research. https://www.understandinganimalresearch.org.uk/news/human-diseases-are-threatening-chimpanzees. Last updated 12 February 2024.
- Slocombe, K.E., Townsend, S.W. and Zuberbühler, K., 2009. Wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) distinguish between different scream types: evidence from a playback study. Animal cognition, 12, pp.441-449.
- Takemoto, H., 2004. Seasonal change in terrestriality of chimpanzees in relation to microclimate in the tropical forest. American Journal of Physical Anthropology: The Official Publication of the American Association of Physical Anthropologists, 124(1), pp.81-92.
- Tutin, C.E., 1979. Mating patterns and reproductive strategies in a community of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Behavioral ecology and sociobiology, 6, pp.29-38.
- Wakefield, M.L., 2008. Grouping patterns and competition among female Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 29, pp.907-929.
- Wroblewski, E. E., Murray, C. M., Keele, B. F., Schumacher-Stankey, J. C., Hahn, B. H., & Pusey, A. E. (2009). Male dominance rank and reproductive success in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Animal Behaviour, 77(4), 873. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.014
- Zaccarella, E., Bortolato, T., Friederici, A. D., Wittig, R. M., & Crockford, C. (2022). Chimpanzees produce diverse vocal sequences with ordered and recombinatorial properties. Communications Biology, 5(1), 1-15. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-03350-8
Written by Acima Cherian, February 2025